Understanding risk and lessons for clinical risk communication about treatment preferences ========================================================================================== * A Edwards * G Elwyn ## Abstract This paper defines risk and its component elements and describes where clinical practice may be starting from in terms of what is reported in the literature about understanding risks and the information requirements of consumers. It notes briefly how theoretical models in the literature contribute to our understanding by providing a basis from which to summarise current evidence about the effects of healthcare interventions which address risks and risk behaviour. The situations or types of interventions in which risk related interventions are most effective are described, but a significant caveat is noted about the types of outcomes which have been reported in the literature and which are most appropriate to evaluate. The effects of “framing” variations in the information given to consumers and the ethical dilemmas these raise for a debate about “informed choice” in healthcare programmes are discussed. In response to both the practical and ethical dilemmas that arise from the current evidence, some of the areas where attention should be focused in the future are outlined so that both health gain and informed choice might be achieved. These include the use of decision aids, although their implementation is not widespread at present. Lessons from the current literature on how further progress can be made towards improved communication, discussion between professionals and consumers, and enhancing informed choice are discussed. * patient preference * risk * informed choice * decision making * patient-caregiver communication ### **Key messages** * Greater flexibility is required in matching information about treatments or care to the needs of individual consumers. * The potential to manipulate consumer decisions by framing manipulations is great and should be minimised. * Clinicians, researchers, and policy makers should note the potential for more informed choices by individual consumers to result in lesser or greater use of services; in some situations this may be less likely to maximise health gain in the population. * Decision aids are being developed widely and offer scope to help achieve informed decision making and greater involvement of consumers in their health care. Continuing evaluations are required, assessing their effects on health outcomes, resource use, and whether informed choices and satisfaction with decision making have been achieved. ## Defining risk Risk is the probability that a hazard will give rise to harm.1 Naturally, therefore, when thinking about how risks are interpreted, two elements must be addressed. The first is the probability of it happening. How the language or statistics are perceived by the individual may vary, and some of the contributory factors to this variation will be discussed below. The term “subjective probability” describes this component of risk interpretation.2 The second component concerns the actual harm, and its severity for that individual. The importance or value placed on the adverse event, such as developing breast cancer, may also vary from one individual to another according to their knowledge and personal experiences, and these values may be termed “outcome utilities”.3 Other terms used for the concept of severity include “adversity” and “burden”.34 From this abstract angle, it appears that discussion of risks and provision of information to healthcare users should address both the probabilistic aspects and the importance and nature of the adverse events being described. However, putting this into practice is rather more complex, and we will examine what is currently reported in the literature about consumers' information needs and the content of risk related discussions in clinical practice as they seek to express their treatment preferences. ## The starting point Some healthcare professionals spend much of their time discussing the harms and benefits of treatments with their “consumers”,5 which can take the form of describing the broad advantages or disadvantages of different options. Alternatively, it may involve the specific use of numerical data about the potential outcomes of choosing one treatment or another.6 In reality, data are rarely available to professionals when needed,7 so the relevant information is often not being used to maximum effect. Even when the information is available, professionals are unclear about how best to discuss the harms and benefits of treatment most effectively with users.7 There are certainly great risks of misleading users which depend on how the information is presented.8 Addressing these difficulties is crucial if quality in healthcare consultations is to be enhanced. ### INFLUENCE OF RISK INFORMATION ON CONSUMERS There is relatively little theoretical work specifically relating to communication about risks in the healthcare setting, although some practical work has been done. The interpretation of risks varies greatly,9 with wide ranges in the meanings or numerical values attributed to verbal descriptions of risks (“rarely”, “sometimes”, “often”, and so on). For example, the term “frequent” was expressed on average as equivalent to nearly 70% in one study looking at information about the probabilities of harm and benefit from treatments, but with a wide range around this figure of 30–90%.10 Wide variations in the interpretation of numerical data among physicians have also been described.11 Studies in the literature suggest that most people usually prefer numerical presentation of information, but approximately one third of consumers prefer verbal descriptions.1213 The type of information preferred and how people understand numerical information is affected by several factors, including the severity of the illness or other outcome concerned, and characteristics such as age, educational level, health status, and recent experience of illness.14 Single figures presented in isolation—for example, one in 10 000—may be interpreted differently from when presented in a list of sequential risks. Presenting single figures without others with which to compare them may lead to overweighting of low probabilities and underweighting of high probabilities.15 Furthermore, people differ not just in their interpretation of the language of risk (different evaluation of the same terms), but also in the meaning or significance they attach to different outcomes.16 The “utilities” or values that people place on different outcomes are likely to affect their use of the risk information in modifying or not modifying their own risks. For example, people's understanding of the term “breast cancer” and the significance they attach to it may affect the degree to which they are motivated to choose to enter screening programmes, even if the same information is presented to all such consumers. ### THEORETICAL MODELS IN THE LITERATURE There is a considerable theoretical base in the literature for understanding risk (see also paper by Lloyd in this supplement17). The usual conceptual framework for this derives from two main strands—cognitive psychology and decision making theory. In general, the models seek to provide an understanding of how individuals perceive risk and how this influences behaviour. These models frequently attribute consequences in behaviour change to two underlying dimensions: an individual's perception of the *value* of an outcome presented in a health recommendation and the perceived *threat* presented by the outcomes in the recommendation. The Health Belief Model (HBM), the Theory of Reasoned Action, the Theory of Social Behaviour, and the Prospect Theory all emphasise the perceived value of a presented consequence.1819 The Transtheoretical Model (“stages of change”) is another model in which interpretation of the likelihood of behaviour change is understood in terms of an individual's readiness to change, and interventions may be targeted accordingly.20 Many of these models are indeed the basis for planning several risk communication interventions. ## Effects of interventions To summarise the effects of interventions addressing risk and risk behaviour, we refer to a systematic review of the literature.21 This review sought to identify effective risk communication interventions and then to identify the characteristics of the most effective interventions—the “effect modifiers”. Ninety seven studies were included in the review. Modest beneficial effects of the interventions were seen across a range of clinical topics (mean effect size 0.3; funnel plot midline of effect sizes approximately 0.15). This is equivalent, for example, to a study demonstrating that adherence to a screening programme increased from 70% to 83% with the introduction of a risk communication intervention. ### MOST EFFECTIVE RISK COMMUNICATION Two key “effect modifiers” were identified—namely, “treatment choice” clinical topics and the use of individualised (calculated) risk estimates in the risk communication process. The treatment choices included topics such as cholesterol lowering therapy, blood pressure therapy, and hormone replacement therapy. Risk communication interventions were more effective in these situations, where consumers were making decisions or expressing treatment preferences, than in studies which attempted to modify risk behaviour such as uptake of screening tests or smoking cessation. ### GOALS OF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS In the treatment choices studied, professionals may often be close to “equipoise”—that is, not having a clear preference about which treatment (or no treatment) the consumer chooses. In risk behaviour modification programmes, professionals often have a clear aim—namely, to enhance uptake of tests or reduce risk exposure. This does not sit comfortably with the notion of enhancing “informed choice”22 but it is an important dilemma that should be resolved. To date, studies commonly report changes in perhaps more objective outcomes such as behavioural or health outcome measures.23 In general, the studies have not evaluated whether risk communication has achieved improvements in understanding among consumers. In keeping with the spirit of partnership and “evidence based patient choice” now emerging,2425 perhaps the professional goals of communication should be to enable “informed choices” by consumers rather than simply to modify behaviour. This may be in spite of the fact that conventional public health gains from these newer approaches could be smaller in some situations. There is a potential conflict between allowing greater choice for consumers and some healthcare policy that is directed more towards standardised healthcare provision. Resolving this conflict will require explicit debate between the stakeholders with recognition of the implications. Wider consumer involvement may result in greater variations in treatment or care provided/used, but variations in care have also been associated with health care that is not evidence based. Attempts to make healthcare provision more standard may need to be afforded a lower priority if wider consumer involvement is to be promoted. In the literature review referred to above,21 individually calculated risk estimates (based on personal risk factors) were most effective in achieving improved patient outcomes. However, the studies only addressed a narrow range of clinical topics including calculating individual breast cancer risks from the Gail formula26 or cardiovascular risks, usually from the Framingham study data.27 Further research should examine the generalisability of these findings for other clinical topics. If they do appear to be generalisable, attention should then be paid to the ways in which information is used and presented in health care practice. ### FRAMING For any method of risk communication, different ways of “framing” the information have varying effects.82228 Framing itself is defined as presenting “logically equivalent” information in different ways.18 For example, the risk of major osteoporotic fractures is 12% in women who take hormone replacement therapy (HRT) for over 5 years and 15% in those who do not. This can be framed as a 3% reduction in (absolute) risk or that fractures are 20% less common in women who take treatment (relative risk reduction). Other framing variations include expressing the figures as “3% more people remaining free of fractures with HRT” (positive framing) or “3% more people suffering fractures if not taking HRT” (negative framing), or converting absolute risk reductions into the “number needed to screen”.29 These different expressions have different motivational effects and substantially influence whether individuals choose treatment options or adhere to chosen plans.2230 There are clear risks of manipulating consumer decisions by the way information is presented,28 thus restricting opportunities for informed choice. Care is therefore crucial in deciding whether to use such formats in discussions with consumers, whether it is truly helpful, or whether the formats should only be used in research and policy settings. People have different preferences for the way they wish information to be presented and discussed with them. For example, some people may not be comfortable with the use of numerical terms and may prefer the same facts to be conveyed descriptively, such as “fractures are slightly less common in those who take HRT”. These issues are examined further by Dudley elsewhere in this supplement.31 ## The future The way facts and figures are expressed ought to vary according to the needs of the individual consumer. As mentioned above, some may prefer more descriptive terms, building up scenarios to illustrate what the professionals mean to convey.32 Others may prefer numerical data, and others still may prefer graphical formats. From the professional's perspective, one such approach alone is also likely to be insufficient for clinical practice.33 Professionals usually wish to be able to choose the method or presentation format for information being used in a consultation to meet the needs of the individual consumer.7 Having identified substantial differences in treatment choices made by consumers when presented with absolute or relative risk information, Hux and Naylor concluded that “multiple complementary formats may be most appropriate”30 and this is supported by other workers.34 A range of complementary formats—for example, descriptive, absolute and relative risk, “numbers needed to treat”, and graphical presentations—may be more valued by professionals and patients.33 Having such information available may facilitate partnerships between professionals and consumers in the consultation, in which both are able to make an informed contribution. Whatever data are available, it is important to maintain a sense of perspective.35 Thus, some have suggested that absolute risk should be the preferred format for presentation of data.8 This may be a balanced perspective of results, but both relative and absolute risk information in isolation can be criticised as only giving part of the picture. People often make decisions on the basis of making comparisons,36 and this requires relative risk as well as absolute risk data. Others have advocated using “everyday risks” with which people are familiar (such as car driving or others as appropriate) to maintain an accurate perspective of the size of a risk and to provide data from which people are able to make an informed comparison of the risk.137 ### ETHICAL PRINCIPLES Such approaches may enable patients to make informed choices based on the “whole truth” rather than the “truth”. In terms of ethical principles, this appears to be closer to the “relationality” principle proposed by Bottorff *et al*.38 It complements other ethical principles such as (consumer) autonomy, beneficence, non-maleficence, and justice. Relationality promotes the provision of accurate honest information in the context of the individual situation. It examines the ethics of care in terms of such factors as response, interpretation, accountability, and social solidarity, often counterbalanced against other values such as truth and confidentiality. If the relevant information is made available in this context, then perhaps some strides are being made towards “informed choice” and thus achievement of quality in healthcare consultations. Further discussion of the ethics of informed and shared decision making is covered elsewhere in this supplement.39 ### EVIDENCE OF INFORMED CHOICE AND UNDERSTANDING Such informed choice may not be evident in the conventional cognitive and behavioural outcomes40 frequently reported in the literature. It is perhaps more likely to be evident in the affective outcomes which are increasingly reported.21 These include satisfaction with the communication process, understanding of the risks and benefits of the different options, and certainty that the best treatment choice has been made.23 Many of these outcomes are now the focus of attention for current work in this field41 and may be of greater importance to consumers. How much consumers understand of the disclosed information is also an important issue, and ethicists have argued that consumers must “substantially understand” information when giving consent to treatment or tests.42 The ethical debate has perhaps not progressed to a point where the notion of “substantial understanding” has been clarified.42 Others have explored in practice, though, what can be learnt about the value of the “reasonable person's standard” in comparison with the “professional standard”.43 Whatever the level of understanding, adhering to the chosen treatment is likely to be a very different prospect for those who are satisfied and “certain” compared with those who are not and who are still in a position of “decisional conflict”.44 We therefore suggest that future strategies to enhance informed choice should be based on methods which portray the decision issues in more depth and which use individual risk estimates. The information should be presented (framed) in as fair and balanced a way as reasonably possible, set in the context where appropriate of everyday risks with which the consumer is familiar. From the discussion about framing effects, it seems that a range of complementary data formats should be available to professionals.30 This could be referred to as a “shopping basket” of options with enough flexibility to address the needs of a great range in requirements of consumers. This idea is starting to be operationalised in some of the “decision aids” now available,41 including booklets, tapes, videodiscs, interactive computer programmes, and paper based charts. Some decision aids are provided for consumers to work through on their own (outside the consultation or at home),3445–47 some are specifically intended to provide a platform for discussions in a further consultation,48–50 while others form the basis for discussions within consultations, prompting consumer questions and so on.5152 Each is likely to have its place according to the situation, but where decision aids are discussed in consultations, this is perhaps more likely to ensure that both patient and professional contribute to the process of making an informed decision (“shared decision making”53). Additional components of decision aids include structured counselling approaches for the professional,54 exploration of the consumer's preferred level of involvement in the decision making itself,4955 and the use of specific approaches to clarify or quantify consumer values—for example, “weigh scales”49 or formal utility assessment methods.56 Value clarification exercises may be highly relevant to attempts to achieve and demonstrate that informed choice has occurred. It is noteworthy that, when provided with information and opportunity for greater involvement in decisions, consumers generally become more wary of the treatments offered and make more conservative choices. In their review of this specific area of the literature, O'Connor *et al*4157 found that, on average, consumers were 26% less likely to choose treatment or tests in studies where information was provided and choices offered (relative risk of choosing treatment 0.74, 95% CI 0.6 to 0.9). Such findings are not direct evidence of understanding the risk and other information provided, but where the direction of effects is contrary to the simplistic goals of interventions (for example, greater uptake of tests), this is at least suggestive that consumers have grasped onto certain key elements of the information and that this has affected the choices made. These effects are likely to be of great interest not just to consumer representative groups, but also to purchasers of care; decision aids may be highly cost effective interventions. Such findings may change, however, as further trials address a broader range of healthcare choices, particularly those in which apparent “overutilisation” of health care is less clear cut. ### IMPLEMENTATION Many professionals are reluctant to use some of the decision aids and other tools now becoming available.5859 Wider implementation is likely to depend on greater promotion of the principles of “shared decision making” so that professional attitudinal barriers may be diminished.5358 This stimulus may be most effective if it comes from consumers or their representatives, rather than professional opinion leaders. Professionals will also need to be sufficiently familiar with the content of decision aids to use them to maximum benefit. Interactive media may be required to enable individually calculated risks to be used in consultations. This presents practical difficulties of using technological innovations across a range of clinical settings, and suggests that interventions must remain simple if they are to be broadly implemented. For both simple and complex decision aids, professionals will require training, not least to feel confident in their own understanding and ability to use risk information.7 It is also important for any training to address the ethical issues raised (see above), particularly relating to the risk of manipulating consumer decisions or behaviour. Raising awareness of these issues should be the first step at least towards reducing the risks of professionals manipulating individual consumers with data. In this context, there will be a platform from which to discuss risks, to enhance understanding of the risks and potential risk reductions, and to facilitate informed choices by consumers. ## References 1. Mohanna K, Chambers R. Risk: what's that all about then? In: Mohanna K, Chambers R, eds. *Risk matters in health care: communicating, explaining and managing risk*. 1st ed. Abingdon, Oxon: Radcliffe Medical Press, 2001: 3–14. 2. Shiloh S. Genetic counselling: a developing area of interest for psychologists. Professional Psychol Res Pract 1996;27:475–86. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037//0735-7028.27.5.475&link_type=DOI) 3. Palmer CG, Sainfort F. Toward a new conceptualization and operationalization of risk perception within the genetic counseling domain. J Genet Counsel 1993;2:275–94. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/BF00961576&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=24242749&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) 4. Frets PG, Niermeijer MF. Reproductive planning after genetic counselling: a perspective from the last decade. Clin Genet 1990;38:295–306. [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=2268975&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) [Web of Science](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1990EE85400008&link_type=ISI) 5. Makoul G, Arntson P, Schofield T. Health promotion in primary care: physician-patient communication and decision making about prescription medications. Soc Sci Med 1995;41:1241–54. 6. Jacoby A, Baker G, Chadwick D, *et al*. The impact of counselling with a practical statistical model on patients' decision-making about treatment for epilepsy: findings from a pilot study. Epilepsy Res 1993;16:207–14. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0920-1211(93)90081-H&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8119271&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) [Web of Science](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1993MK99900004&link_type=ISI) 7. Edwards AGK, Matthews E, Pill RM, *et al*. Communication about risk: diversity among primary care professionals. Fam Pract 1998;15:296–300. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6ODoiZmFtcHJhY3QiO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6ODoiMTUvNC8yOTYiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoyMzoiL3FoYy8xMC9zdXBwbF8xL2k5LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 8. Skolbekken JA. Communicating the risk reduction achieved by cholesterol reducing drugs. BMJ 1998;316:1956–8. [FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiRlVMTCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoiYm1qIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEzOiIzMTYvNzE0OS8xOTU2IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MjM6Ii9xaGMvMTAvc3VwcGxfMS9pOS5hdG9tIjt9czo4OiJmcmFnbWVudCI7czowOiIiO30=) 9. Mazur DJ. Interpretation of graphic data by patients in a general medical clinic. J Gen Intern Med 1990;5:402–5. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/BF02599425&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=2231035&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) [Web of Science](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1990EA37000004&link_type=ISI) 10. Woloshin KK, Ruffin MTI, Gorenflo DW. Patients' interpretation of qualitative probability statements. Arch Fam Med 1994;3:961–6. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archfami.1994.01850240041009&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=7804478&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) 11. Bryant GD, Norman GR. Expressions of probability: words and numbers. N Engl J Med 1980;302:411. [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=7351943&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) [Web of Science](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1980JE27800025&link_type=ISI) 12. Freeman TR, Bass MJ. Risk language preferred by mothers in considering a hypothetical new vaccine for their children. Can Med Assoc J 1992;147:1013–7. [Abstract](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NDoiY21haiI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czoxMDoiMTQ3LzcvMTAxMyI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 13. Shaw NJ, Dear PRF. How do parents of babies interpret qualitative expressions of probability. Arch Dis Child 1990;65:520–3. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MTI6ImFyY2hkaXNjaGlsZCI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czo4OiI2NS81LzUyMCI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 14. Mazur DJ, Merz JF. Patients interpretations of verbal expressions of probability: implications for securing informed consent to medical interventions. Behav Sci Law 1994;12:417–26. [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10150709&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) [Web of Science](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1994PV26600009&link_type=ISI) 15. Shiloh S, Sagi M. Effect of framing on the perception of genetic recurrence risks. Am J Med Genet 1989;33:130–5. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/ajmg.1320330121&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=2750781&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) [Web of Science](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1989AC65300020&link_type=ISI) 16. Edwards AGK, Pill RM, Stott NCH. Communicating risk: use of standard terms is unlikely to result in standard communication. BMJ 1996;313:1483. [FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiRlVMTCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoiYm1qIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEzOiIzMTMvNzA3MC8xNDgzIjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MjM6Ii9xaGMvMTAvc3VwcGxfMS9pOS5hdG9tIjt9czo4OiJmcmFnbWVudCI7czowOiIiO30=) 17. Lloyd AJ. The extent of patients' understanding of the risk of treatments. Quality in Health Care 2001;10(Suppl I):i14–18. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoicWhjIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjE0OiIxMC9zdXBwbF8xL2kxNCI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 18. Wilson DK, Purdon SE, Wallston KA. Compliance to health recommendations: a theoretical overview of message framing. Health Educ Res 1988;3:161–71. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoiaGVyIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjc6IjMvMi8xNjEiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoyMzoiL3FoYy8xMC9zdXBwbF8xL2k5LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 19. Becker MH, Shumaker SA, Eleanor B. Theoretical models of adherence and strategies for improving adherence. In: Schron JK, Ockene CT, Parker JL, *et al*, eds. *The handbook of health behavior change.* New York: Springer, 1990: 5–43. 20. Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC. Stages of change in the modification of problem behaviors. Prog Behav Modif 1992;28:183–218. [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=1620663&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) 21. Edwards AGK, Hood K, Matthews EJ, *et al*. The effectiveness of one-to-one risk communication interventions in health care: a systematic review. Med Decis Making 2000;20:290–7. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NToic3BtZG0iO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6ODoiMjAvMy8yOTAiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoyMzoiL3FoYy8xMC9zdXBwbF8xL2k5LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 22. Sarfati D, Howden-Chapman P, Woodward, *et al*. Does the frame affect the picture? A study into how attitudes to screening for cancer are affected by the way benefits are expressed. J Med Screening 1998;5:137–40. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NToic3Btc2MiO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6NzoiNS8zLzEzNyI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 23. Edwards AGK, Elwyn GJ. How should `effectiveness' of risk communication to aid patients' decisions be judged? A review of the literature. Med Decis Making 1999;19:428–34. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NToic3BtZG0iO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6ODoiMTkvNC80MjgiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoyMzoiL3FoYy8xMC9zdXBwbF8xL2k5LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 24. Richards T. Partnership with patients. BMJ 1998;316:85–6. [FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiRlVMTCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoiYm1qIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjExOiIzMTYvNzEyNS84NSI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 25. Hope T. *Evidence-based patient choice*. London: King's Fund, 1996. 26. Gail MH, Brinton LA, Byar DP, *et al*. Projecting individualised probabilities of developing breast cancer for white females who are being examined annually. J Natl Cancer Inst 1989;81:1879–86. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NDoiam5jaSI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czoxMDoiODEvMjQvMTg3OSI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 27. Wilson PWF, D'Agostino RB, Levy D, *et al*. Prediction of coronary heart disease using risk factor categories. Circulation 1998;97:1837–47. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MTQ6ImNpcmN1bGF0aW9uYWhhIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEwOiI5Ny8xOC8xODM3IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MjM6Ii9xaGMvMTAvc3VwcGxfMS9pOS5hdG9tIjt9czo4OiJmcmFnbWVudCI7czowOiIiO30=) 28. Edwards AGK, Elwyn G, Mathews E, *et al*. Presenting risk information: a review of the effects of `framing' and other manipulations on patient outcomes. J Health Commun 2001;6:61–82. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1080/10810730150501413&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11317424&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) [Web of Science](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000166987900005&link_type=ISI) 29. Rembold C. Number needed to screen: development of a statistic for disease screening. BMJ 2000;317:307–12. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoiYm1qIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEyOiIzMTcvNzE1NC8zMDciO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoyMzoiL3FoYy8xMC9zdXBwbF8xL2k5LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 30. Hux JE, Naylor CD. Communicating the benefits of chronic preventive therapy: does the format of efficacy data determine patients' acceptance of treatment? Med Decis Making 1995;15:152–7. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NToic3BtZG0iO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6ODoiMTUvMi8xNTIiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoyMzoiL3FoYy8xMC9zdXBwbF8xL2k5LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 31. Dudley N. The importance of risk communciation and decision making in three cardiovascular conditions: a discussion paper. Quality Health Care 2001;10(Suppl I):i19–22. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoicWhjIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjE0OiIxMC9zdXBwbF8xL2kxOSI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 32. Greenhalgh T. Narrative based medicine: narrative based medicine in an evidence based world. BMJ 2000;318:323–5. [FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiRlVMTCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoiYm1qIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEyOiIzMTgvNzE3OS8zMjMiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoyMzoiL3FoYy8xMC9zdXBwbF8xL2k5LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 33. Edwards AGK, Matthews EJ, Pill RM, *et al*. Communication about risk: the responses of primary care professionals to standardising the language of risk and communication tools. Fam Pract 1998;15:301–7. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6ODoiZmFtcHJhY3QiO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6ODoiMTUvNC8zMDEiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoyMzoiL3FoYy8xMC9zdXBwbF8xL2k5LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 34. Liao L, Jollis JG, DeLong ER, *et al*. Impact of an interactive video on decision making of patients with ischaemic heart disease. J Gen Intern Med 1996;11:373–6. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/BF02600051&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8803746&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) [Web of Science](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1996UT65100010&link_type=ISI) 35. Edwards AGK, Elwyn GJ, Stott NCH. Researchers should present results with both relative and absolute risks. BMJ 1999;318:603–4. 36. Jarvenpaa SL. The effect of task demands and graphical format on information processing strategies. Manage Sci 1989;35:285–303. 37. Paling J. *Up to your armpits in alligators: how to sort out what risks are worth worrying about.* 1st ed. Gainesville, Florida: Risk Communication & Environmental Institute, 1997. 38. Bottorff JD, Ratner PA, Johnson JL. *Uncertainties and challenges. Communicating risk in the context of familial cancer*. Report to the National Cancer Institute of Canada, Vancouver, School of Nursing, University of British Columbia, 1996. 39. Doyal L. Informed consent: moral necessity or illusion? Quality in Health Care 2001;10(Suppl I):i29–33. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoicWhjIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjE0OiIxMC9zdXBwbF8xL2kyOSI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 40. Llewellyn-Thomas HA. Patients' health care decision-making: a framework for descriptive and experimental investigations. Med Decis Making 1995;15:101–6. [FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6MzoiUERGIjtzOjExOiJqb3VybmFsQ29kZSI7czo1OiJzcG1kbSI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czo4OiIxNS8yLzEwMSI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 41. O'Connor A, Fiset V, Rosto A, *et al*. Decision aids for people facing health treatment or screening decisions. In: *Cochrane Collection*. Cochrane Library. Issue 1. Oxford: Update Software, 1999. 42. Mazur DJ. Information disclosure and beyond? How do patients understand and use the information they report they want? Med Decis Making 2000;20:132–3. [FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6MzoiUERGIjtzOjExOiJqb3VybmFsQ29kZSI7czo1OiJzcG1kbSI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czo4OiIyMC8xLzEzMiI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 43. Van Haecht CHM, Vanderstichele R, Debacker G, *et al*. Impact of patient package inserts on patient satisfaction, adverse drug-reactions and risk perception: the case of NSAIDs for posttraumatic pain relief. Patient Educ Counsel 1991;17:205–15. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0738-3991(91)90061-9&link_type=DOI) 44. O'Connor AM. Validation of a decisional conflict scale. Med Decis Making 1995;15:25–30. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NToic3BtZG0iO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6NzoiMTUvMS8yNSI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 45. Sawka CA, Goel V, Mahut CA, *et al*. Development of a patient decision aid for choice of surgical treatment for breast cancer. Health Expect 1998;1:23–36. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1046/j.1369-6513.1998.00003.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11281859&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) 46. Bernstein SJ, Skarupski KA, Grayson CE, *et al*. A randomized controlled trial of information-giving to patients referred for coronary angiography: effects on outcomes of care. Health Expect 1998;1:50–61. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1046/j.1369-6513.1998.00007.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11281861&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) 47. O'Connor AM, Tugwell P, Wells GA, *et al*. Randomized trial of a portable, self-administered decision aid for postmenopausal women considering long-term hormone therapy. Med Decis Making 1998;18:295–303. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NToic3BtZG0iO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6ODoiMTgvMy8yOTUiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoyMzoiL3FoYy8xMC9zdXBwbF8xL2k5LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 48. Davison BJ, Degner LF. Empowerment of men newly diagnosed with prostate cancer. Cancer Nurs 1997;20:187–96. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/00002820-199706000-00004&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9190093&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) [Web of Science](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1997XE57200004&link_type=ISI) 49. Man-Son-Hing M, Laupacis A, O'Connor AM, *et al*. A randomized controlled trial of a decision aid for patients with atrial fibrillation. JAMA 1999;282:737–43. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/jama.282.8.737&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10463708&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) [Web of Science](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000082118800024&link_type=ISI) 50. Street RL, van Order A, Bramson R, *et al*. Preconsultation education promoting breast cancer screening: does the choice of media make a difference? J Cancer Educ 1998;13:152–61. [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10898560&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) 51. Whelan TJ, Levine M, Gafni A, *et al*. Breast irradiation postlumpectomy: development and evaluation of a decision instrument. J Clin Oncol 1995;13:847–53. [Abstract](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoiamNvIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjg6IjEzLzQvODQ3IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MjM6Ii9xaGMvMTAvc3VwcGxfMS9pOS5hdG9tIjt9czo4OiJmcmFnbWVudCI7czowOiIiO30=) 52. Holmes-Rovner M, Kroll J, Rovner D, *et al*. Patient decision support intervention: increased consistency with decision analytic models. Med Care 1999;37:270–84. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/00005650-199903000-00007&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10098571&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom) [Web of Science](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000079250000007&link_type=ISI) 53. Elwyn GJ, Edwards AGK, Kinnersley P. Shared decision making in primary care: the neglected second half of the consultation. Br J Gen Pract 1999;49:477–82. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NDoiYmpncCI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czoxMDoiNDkvNDQzLzQ3NyI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 54. Lerman C, Biesecker B, Benkendorf JL, *et al*. Controlled trial of pretest education approaches to enhance informed decision-making for BRCA1 gene testing. J Natl Cancer Inst 1997;89:148–57. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NDoiam5jaSI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czo4OiI4OS8yLzE0OCI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 55. Robinson A, Thomson R. Variability in patient preferences for participating in medical decision making: implication for the use of decision support tools. Quality in Health Care 2001;10(Suppl I):i34–8. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoicWhjIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjE0OiIxMC9zdXBwbF8xL2kzNCI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjIzOiIvcWhjLzEwL3N1cHBsXzEvaTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 56. Protheroe J, Fahey T, Montgomery A. The impact of patients' preferences on the treatment of atrial fibrillation: observational study of patient based decision analysis. BMJ 2000;320:1380–4. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoiYm1qIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEzOiIzMjAvNzI0Ni8xMzgwIjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MjM6Ii9xaGMvMTAvc3VwcGxfMS9pOS5hdG9tIjt9czo4OiJmcmFnbWVudCI7czowOiIiO30=) 57. O'Connor AM, Rostom A, Fiset V, *et al*. Decision aids for patients facing health treatment or screening decisions: systematic review. BMJ 1999;319:731–4. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoiYm1qIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEyOiIzMTkvNzIxMi83MzEiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoyMzoiL3FoYy8xMC9zdXBwbF8xL2k5LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 58. Elwyn GJ, Edwards AGK, Gwyn R, *et al*. Towards a feasible model for shared decision-making: focus group study with general practice registrars. BMJ 1999;319:753–6. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MzoiYm1qIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEyOiIzMTkvNzIxMi83NTMiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoyMzoiL3FoYy8xMC9zdXBwbF8xL2k5LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 59. Holmes-Rovner M, Valade D, Orlowski C, *et al*. Implementing shared decision-making in routine practice: barriers and opportunities. Health Expect 2000;3:182–91. [CrossRef](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1046/j.1369-6513.2000.00093.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://qualitysafety.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11281928&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fqhc%2F10%2Fsuppl_1%2Fi9.atom)