Main

The possible influence of delays in diagnosis on survival and the risk factors for delay in patients with cancer have been the subject of considerable interest and controversy for many years. Survival from cancer in the United Kingdom is poorer than that of other European countries, and it has been suggested that this can be attributed to more advanced disease stage at presentation (Cancer Research UK, 2009). Consequently, there is a need to understand the diagnostic process, and to ascertain risk factors related to increased time to presentation and treatment. This is particularly relevant in the context of primary care, where many patients present with symptoms that may be indicative of cancer, but where the outcome of the diagnostic process is the exclusion of cancer in most cases. Furthermore, in countries with strong primary healthcare systems, such as the United Kingdom, this is typically the first point of contact for the majority of patients, making early recognition of cancer symptoms even more pertinent.

Minimising time to diagnosis of cancer is dependent on patients presenting with potential cancer symptoms and on primary care practitioners responding appropriately to those symptoms, either by arranging further investigation and/or referring for specialist input. Delay can occur at three phases during the diagnostic process: first, in the interval between the patient first noticing a symptom and first consulting a doctor (often referred to as patient delay), second, between first consultation and referral by a practitioner (doctor or practitioner delay) and finally, between referral and diagnosis (hospital or system delay) (Nichols et al, 1981).

In addition to outlining the various points during which extraneous factors might adversely influence time to presentation and referral, considering delay in these phases enables identification of areas where interventions designed to reduce delay might be targeted. However, before such interventions can be developed, we need to better understand the factors that might lengthen or shorten the time taken by patients to seek help for symptoms and gain greater insights into how practitioners (usually in primary care) respond to these symptoms. This topic is not easily amenable to study by randomised trial, and as such, interpreting data available from observational studies is essential to increasing our understanding of the risk factors associated with delay and to facilitating development of effective new strategies to reduce the time involved. If longer delays do impact on survival, as has been shown for breast cancer (Richards et al, 1999), such strategies could save a significant number of lives.

Materials and methods

We report results from two systematic reviews of the world literature. The first relates specifically to breast cancer (Ramirez et al, 1999; Westcombe et al, 1999). The second covers other common cancers, namely colorectal, gynaecological, lung, upper gastrointestinal and urological (Macdonald et al, 2004, 2006; Mitchell et al, 2008). Both of these reviews described the extent of patient-mediated and practitioner-mediated delays and identified the factors that may cause such delays. The breast cancer literature was reviewed from 1966 to 1997, and 23 studies met the inclusion criteria; all were assessed as being of sufficient quality for inclusion (Ramirez et al, 1999). Literature relating to the other cancers was reviewed from 1970 to 2003, and 54 studies met the inclusion criteria for colorectal cancer (45 sufficient quality), 16 for gynaecological cancers (12 sufficient quality), 8 for lung cancer (4 sufficient quality), 25 for upper gastrointestinal cancers (18 sufficient quality) and 16 for urological cancers (11 sufficient quality). Taken together. therefore, 142 papers were reviewed, of which 113 were deemed of sufficient quality for inclusion in the analyses. Detailed methods of each review are described elsewhere, including the process of determining quality (Westcombe et al, 1999; Macdonald et al, 2004). This paper reports on and discusses risk factors for patient and primary care practitioner delay. It combines and summarises earlier reported results for breast, colorectal and upper gastrointestinal cancers, with previously unreported results for gynaecological, lung and urological cancers. Only risk factors derived from studies of sufficient quality are presented; these are reported as either increasing or reducing delay or as inconclusive, that is the evidence was neither in support of the factor predominately increasing nor reducing delay, and as such, the direction of influence cannot be determined definitively.

Terminology

The term ‘delay’ in the cancer literature is used both to refer to time delays and to denote advanced stage at presentation. More recently, authors have tended to avoid use of the word delay, not only because of this confusion, but also because the term implies an active decision for inaction. However, the term is used extensively in the literature and as such is impossible to avoid in a report of this nature. Any reference to delay in this paper refers to time delay.

Results

Risk factors for patient delay

Demographic factors

Several demographic factors have been studied in relation to their association with presentation for cancer symptoms.

Age Older patient age was found to be a risk factor for delayed presentation with symptoms of breast cancer (Ramirez et al, 1999), but was unrelated to delay for colorectal cancer (Mitchell et al, 2008), urological cancers (Thornhill et al, 1987; Samet et al, 1988; Mansson et al, 1993; Risberg et al, 1996) and lung cancer (Worden and Weisman, 1975; Mor et al, 1990; Risberg et al, 1996). There was no conclusive evidence in relation to the influence of age on presentation with symptoms of upper gastrointestinal (Macdonald et al, 2006) or gynaecological cancers (Fruchter et al, 1980; Franceschi et al, 1983; Fowler et al, 1984; Smith and Anderson, 1985; Dhamija et al, 1993; Andersen et al, 1995; Coates et al, 1996; Risberg et al, 1996; Jones and Joura, 1999; Goff et al, 2000) (Tables 1 and 3, 4 and 5).

Table 1 Risk factors for patient-mediated delay

Gender Overall, men were found to delay longer than women in presenting with bladder cancer (Mommsen et al, 1983; Mansson et al, 1993; Risberg et al, 1996) and with other urological cancers (Risberg et al, 1996). There was little evidence of any association between sex and time to presentation for either upper or lower gastrointestinal cancers (Macdonald et al, 2006; Mitchell et al, 2008) or for lung cancer (Worden and Weisman, 1975; Mor et al, 1990; Risberg et al, 1996; Bowen and Rayner, 2002).

Socio-economic status and education Lower socio-economic status was associated with increased delay by patients presenting with symptoms of upper gastrointestinal cancers (Macdonald et al, 2006) and by men with prostate cancer (Hackett et al, 1973; Samet et al, 1988; Fitzpatrick et al, 1998). However, there was no overall relationship between socio-economic status and delay for colorectal cancer (Mitchell et al, 2008), gynaecological cancers (Fruchter et al, 1980; Fowler et al, 1984; Andersen et al, 1995; Coates et al, 1996; Goff et al, 2000) or lung cancer (Hackett et al, 1973; Worden and Weisman, 1975; Mor et al, 1990). Similarly, although lower educational attainment was associated with greater delay for patients with breast (Ramirez et al, 1999) and colorectal cancers (Mitchell et al, 2008), it was not related to presentation for any of the urological (Mansson et al, 1993; Risberg et al, 1996) or gynaecological cancers (Fruchter et al, 1980; Franceschi et al, 1983; Dhamija et al, 1993; Andersen et al, 1995; Risberg et al, 1996) or for lung cancer (Mor et al, 1990; Risberg et al, 1996).

Ethnicity Non-white ethnic origin was a risk factor for longer delay in presenting with breast (Ramirez et al, 1999) and urological cancers (bladder and prostate) (Samet et al, 1988; Prout et al, 2000), but non-white patients were found to have a shorter time to presentation for stomach cancer (Macdonald et al, 2006). No association was found between ethnicity and time to presentation for gynaecological cancers (cervical and uterine) (Fruchter et al, 1980; Fowler et al, 1984; Coates et al, 1996).

Marital status Marital status or living with a partner was found to be unrelated to presentation patterns for breast, gynaecological, lung, upper gastrointestinal and urological cancers (Worden and Weisman, 1975; Franceschi et al, 1983; Thornhill et al, 1987; Mor et al, 1990; Mansson et al, 1993; Andersen et al, 1995; Fitzpatrick et al, 1998; Ramirez et al, 1999; Macdonald et al, 2006). It was not possible to draw a definitive conclusion in relation to its influence on colorectal cancer (Mitchell et al, 2008).

Clinical factors

Symptom type The type of symptoms with which patients present has a marked impact on presenting behaviour (Tables 1 and 3, 4 and 5). Women diagnosed with breast cancer were more likely to delay if they had an atypical symptom, that is one that did not include a breast lump (Ramirez et al, 1999). Across the cancer groups, patients were typically less likely to delay if they experienced a more serious symptom, such as pain, or an alarming symptom, such as bleeding. This was found to be the case for upper gastrointestinal cancers (Macdonald et al, 2006) and gynaecological cancers (Smith and Anderson, 1985, 1987; Menon et al, 1991; Coates et al, 1996; Goff et al, 2000) for pain associated with colorectal cancer (Mitchell et al, 2008) and for bleeding associated with urological cancers (bladder and prostate) (Mansson et al, 1993; Fitzpatrick et al, 1998; Ho et al, 1998). Conversely, pain was a risk factor for increased delay in patients with urological cancers (Hackett et al, 1973; Attard, 1985; Mansson et al, 1993; Fitzpatrick et al, 1998), whereas the evidence for lung cancer was inconclusive (Hackett et al, 1973; Worden and Weisman, 1975). Perhaps, unsurprisingly, the onset of vague, non-specific, more common or multiple symptoms was likely to increase delay (Andersen et al, 1995; Goff et al, 2000; Fitch et al, 2002; Mitchell et al, 2008). For the majority of cancer groups studied, patients were more likely to present when symptoms were, or became, more incapacitating and impacted on daily life and activities (Hackett et al, 1973; Menon et al, 1991; Andersen et al, 1995; Sanden et al, 2000; Macdonald et al, 2006; Mitchell et al, 2008).

Co-morbidity The presence of co-existing morbidity was associated with reduced delay in patients with upper gastrointestinal cancers (Macdonald et al, 2006) and colorectal cancer (Mitchell et al, 2008). However, this was found to be unrelated to presenting behaviour for lung cancer (Mor et al, 1990). There was evidence that regular visits to medical practitioners, including attendance for routine screening, was associated with shorter delay in patients with gynaecological cancers (uterine and cervical) (Menon et al, 1991; Coates et al, 1996) and prostate cancer (Samet et al, 1988; Fitzpatrick et al, 1998). The impact of a personal or family history of cancer on help-seeking behaviour was investigated across several sites, and in the main, there was no conclusive evidence as to its impact. However, for patients with prostate cancer, having had a previous cancer diagnosis (Samet et al, 1988) or having a relative with cancer (Hackett et al, 1973; Fitzpatrick et al, 1998) was typically associated with reduced time to presentation. Furthermore, there was evidence of a positive relationship between increased delay and having previously had a normal test result or the condition diagnosed as benign (Fruchter et al, 1980; Smith and Anderson, 1985).

Psychosocial factors

Awareness and interpretation of symptoms Symptom awareness, and more particularly patients’ interpretation of symptoms, was a commonly reported theme. Non-recognition of the seriousness of symptoms, sometimes related to lack of knowledge about the disease, was the predominant risk factor for delay reported across all cancer sites (Worden and Weisman, 1975; Fruchter et al, 1980; Bosl et al, 1981; Attard, 1985; Smith and Anderson, 1985, 1987; Cochran et al, 1986; Mor et al, 1990; Menon et al, 1991; Mansson et al, 1993; Andersen et al, 1995; Coates et al, 1996; Ajayi and Adewole, 1998; Gascoigne et al, 1999; Ramirez et al, 1999; de Nooijer et al, 2001; Bowen and Rayner, 2002; Fitch et al, 2002; Khadra and Oakeshott, 2002; Kidanto et al, 2002; Macdonald et al, 2006; Mitchell et al, 2008) (Tables 1 and 3, 4 and 5). Delay was often related to patients adopting a ‘wait and see’ approach, denying or redefining their symptoms in relation to benign disease or self-diagnosing and self-medicating before presentation to a practitioner (Macdonald et al, 2006; Mitchell et al, 2008).

Emotional response Concern related to recognition of a potential cancer symptom was also important in the decision to present. Fear that a symptom was indicative of cancer, or fear of investigation, of treatment, or of powerlessness were also found to be factors in increasing time to presentation for upper and lower gastrointestinal cancers (Macdonald et al, 2006; Mitchell et al, 2008), urological cancers (Hackett et al, 1973; Fitzpatrick et al, 1998; Ho et al, 1998; de Nooijer et al, 2001; Sanden et al, 2000), gynaecological cancers (Fruchter et al, 1980; Smith and Anderson, 1985, 1987) and lung cancer (Hackett et al, 1973; Worden and Weisman, 1975; Mor et al, 1990). Similarly, embarrassment about symptoms resulted in longer delay for patients with colorectal (Mitchell et al, 2008) and urological cancers (testicular and prostate) (Fitzpatrick et al, 1998; Gascoigne et al, 1999).

Support Social support and the availability of advice were influential factors in patients’ decisions to present with cancer symptoms. Patients with breast cancer, who did not disclose their symptoms within a week to someone close to them, were more likely to delay help seeking (Ramirez et al, 1999). For patients with colorectal cancer, social networks were identified as a potentially important factor in reducing delay, when patients either sought advice from or made decisions based on the experience of others (Mitchell et al, 2008). In keeping with this, lower levels of social support were found to be associated with increased delay in women with endometrial cancer (Cochran et al, 1986) (Table 1). However, the availability of social support was unrelated to delay for lung, upper gastrointestinal and urological cancers (Samet et al, 1988; Mor et al, 1990; Bowen and Rayner, 2002; Sanden et al, 2000; Macdonald et al, 2006).

Risk factors for practitioner delay

Patient demographic factors

There was some evidence relating to the impact of certain patient characteristics on practitioners’ referral behaviour (Tables 2, 3, 4 and 5).

Table 2 Risk factors for practitioner-mediated delay
Table 3 Risk factors associated with delay for gynaecological cancers
Table 4 Risk factors associated with delay for lung cancer
Table 5 Risk factors associated with delay for urological cancers

Age and gender Older patients were referred more quickly for symptoms of breast, upper gastrointestinal and colorectal cancers (Ramirez et al, 1999; Macdonald et al, 2006; Mitchell et al, 2008). However, younger patients experienced less delay in referral when presenting with symptoms related to one of the urological cancers (Mansson et al, 1993; Risberg et al, 1996) or to lung cancer (Risberg et al, 1996). There was no conclusive evidence on the direction of effect for patient age and practitioner delay for gynaecological cancers (Fruchter et al, 1980; Fowler et al, 1984; Risberg et al, 1996). Men with upper gastrointestinal cancers (Macdonald et al, 2006) and urological cancer (bladder) (Mommsen et al, 1983; Mansson et al, 1993) were less likely than women to have experienced delayed referral, although the evidence relating to colorectal cancer was inconclusive (Mitchell et al, 2008).

Socio-economic status, education and ethnicity Patients from lower socio-economic groups experienced a shorter wait to referral for upper gastrointestinal cancer (Macdonald et al, 2006), but greater delay than more affluent patients in referral for colorectal cancer (Mitchell et al, 2008). Although socio-economic status was unrelated to referral for symptoms of gynaecological cancers (Fowler et al, 1984; Goff et al, 2000), lower educational attainment was associated with delayed referral in this group, as it was for patients with symptoms of lung and urological cancers (Risberg et al, 1996). There was little evidence on the influence of ethnicity, although non-white women with breast cancer experienced less practitioner delay (Ramirez et al, 1999), whereas this was unrelated to referral for gynaecological cancer (cervical) (Fowler et al, 1984).

Presenting symptom and medical history

As was the case in patient delay, presenting with a breast symptom other than a lump was associated with greater delay by practitioners (Ramirez et al, 1999). Typically, pain as a presenting symptom resulted in shorter time to referral for patients with upper gastrointestinal cancers (Macdonald et al, 2006) and gynaecological cancer (ovarian) (Goff et al, 2000; Fitch et al, 2002). This was also found to be the case for bleeding in upper gastrointestinal cancers (Macdonald et al, 2006) and urological cancer (bladder) (Mommsen et al, 1983; Mansson et al, 1993). Patients with co-existing disease were likely to be referred more quickly for symptoms of colorectal cancer (Mitchell et al, 2008), but the evidence relating to upper gastrointestinal cancers was inconclusive (Macdonald et al, 2006). However, time to referral for colorectal cancer was increased for patients who frequently consulted their general practitioner (Mitchell et al, 2008), and the involvement of multiple care providers resulted in delay for patients with gynaecological cancer (ovarian) (Fowler et al, 1984; Goff et al, 2000). Frequency of attendance was unrelated to referral for upper gastrointestinal cancers (Macdonald et al, 2006).

Practitioner response

The most commonly identified themes associated with delayed referral across the cancer sites related to initial diagnosis and activity of the practitioner. Misdiagnosis, occurring either through treating patients symptomatically or by relating symptoms to a health problem other than cancer, resulted in increased time to referral for breast, colorectal, gynaecological, upper gastrointestinal and urological cancers (Bosl et al, 1981; Attard, 1985; Gascoigne et al, 1999; Jones and Joura, 1999; Ramirez et al, 1999; Goff et al, 2000; Fitch et al, 2002; Macdonald et al, 2006; Mitchell et al, 2008). Although there was support for the existence of a similar pattern in relation to diagnosis of lung cancer (Table 4), the evidence was of relatively poor quality (Pereira et al, 1991; Silva et al, 1992; Bowen and Rayner, 2002; Koyi et al, 2002). Failure to fully or adequately examine patients, use of inappropriate or inadequate tests and receiving or failing to follow-up inconclusive, negative or false negative test results contributed to the delay (Fruchter et al, 1980; Bosl et al, 1981; Hernes et al, 1996; Goff et al, 2000; Macdonald et al, 2006; Mitchell et al, 2008). Similarly, prescribing treatment for benign conditions, such as acid suppression in patients subsequently diagnosed with upper gastrointestinal cancers (Macdonald et al, 2006), or antibiotics in patients with testicular cancer (Bosl et al, 1981) also increased time to referral. There is some evidence to suggest that appropriate referral and use of referral guidelines is associated with reduced delay for upper gastrointestinal cancers (Macdonald et al, 2006) and colorectal cancer (Mitchell et al, 2008).

Discussion

Reasons for delay in presentation by patients with symptoms of cancer and influences on time to referral by practitioners are complex and multi-factorial according to the combined findings of two systematic reviews of the literature on common cancers. The predominant risk factor for patient delay is a lack of interpretation by patients of the serious nature of their symptoms. Across the common cancers, symptom type is predictive of delay in presentation. If a symptom is atypical, or vague in nature, the risk of delayed presentation can be increased. Conversely, if the symptom is more serious or alarming and includes a lump, bleeding or pain, then the risk of delayed presentation is typically reduced. Patients may fail to recognise or appreciate atypical or vague symptoms, which may mediate delayed presentation. Where symptoms are understood and thought to be serious, there is a reduced time to presentation, and whatever the cancer site, the catalyst for presentation is often when a symptom becomes incapacitating or impacts on normal activities, rather than the presence of the symptom alone. However, the difficulty is that common cancer symptoms are often attributable to benign disease. The complexity of this process can be illustrated by considering breast cancer, where there is robust evidence that patients delay less with the well-known symptom of lump, compared with the less recognised non-lump symptoms, which result in greater delay. This contrasts with the evidence for urological cancers, where pain increases delay, perhaps as a result of symptoms being misinterpreted as being due to a benign cause such as cystitis. In other cancers too, such as those of the gastrointestinal tract, potential cancer symptoms can frequently have a benign interpretation.

Patient delays are also influenced by a range of demographic and psychosocial factors. Taken together, lower socio-economic status and education level are risk factors for delayed presentation of several common cancers, but not for others. Similarly, non-white ethnic origin impacts on delayed presentation for some, but not all common cancers. Neither age nor gender is associated with delayed presentation with the exception of breast cancer (older age) and bladder cancer (male sex). Across the common cancers, then demographic risk factors for patient delay seem inconsistent. This may be a consequence of the fact that the cancers have not been equally researched in terms of risk factors for delay. Although the review of the literature for breast cancer was based on 23 studies deemed to be of adequate quality, and the upper and lower gastrointestinal results were based on 18 and 45 studies, respectively, only 11 urological, 12 gynaecological and 4 lung cancer studies were of sufficient quality to be included in the synthesis of evidence. Further, it is possible that some of the factors we have described are not independent of each other; the ways in which they may relate to each other are not clear.

General population surveys in the United Kingdom indicate a widespread lack of awareness of the symptoms of cancer (Grunfeld et al, 2002; McCaffery et al, 2003; Linsell et al, 2008; Robb et al, 2009). These low levels of symptom awareness may partly explain why the type of symptom and recognition of the seriousness of symptoms are consistent risk factors for delayed patient presentation. These surveys also suggest that cancer symptom awareness is poorer among those who are less well educated, those with lower socio-economic status and those from black and minority ethnic groups (Wardle et al, 2001; Grunfeld et al, 2002; Robb et al, 2009). Not only are levels of awareness of cancer symptoms low among the general population, but so too is knowledge of the risk factors for developing cancer. Equally, these surveys report that people hold negative beliefs and attitudes about the benefits of seeking medical help for cancer, which include fear, embarrassment, reluctance to bother the general practitioner and nihilism about cancer treatments (Grunfeld et al, 2002; Robb et al, 2009). These barriers may explain the finding from our review that fear and embarrassment are risk factors for patient delay.

Qualitative research with people diagnosed with cancer further enhances the findings of such surveys. Reported barriers to early help seeking are vague and mild symptoms, absence of pain or lump, belief that the symptom will go away, intermittent symptoms, lack of awareness of cancer risk and previous benign diagnosis (Smith et al, 2005). Competing demands and priorities, fears about cancer treatments and anxieties about ‘bothering the doctor’ have also been identified as issues related to delayed presentation in breast cancer (Burgess et al, 2001). Qualitative interviews have enabled the identification of eventual triggers to help seeking, such as worsening symptoms, new additional symptoms, the presence of a symptom that is recognised as serious or is affecting daily life and the influence of family and friends (Burgess et al, 2001; Smith et al, 2005), findings supported by the evidence presented in this paper.

Overall, research into the risk factors for patient delay indicates that presentation with cancer is not a straightforward or linear process. Knowledge of symptoms and risk may be necessary, but not sufficient to determine help seeking for cancer. People's attitudes, beliefs and social context clearly influence the process of medical help seeking. To develop strategies to reduce patient delays, the risk factors for delayed presentation need to be placed into an explanatory framework. This aids understanding of the complexities of the delay process and highlights where and how interventions could be targeted. A framework for understanding patient delay in breast cancer is described by Bish et al (2005). This model incorporates symptom appraisal, attitudes towards help-seeking and translating intentions to seek help into behaviour. It draws on health psychology models, including self-regulation theory (Leventhal et al, 1984), theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen, 1991) and implementation intentions (Gollwitzer, 1999). A similar model (Andersen et al, 1995) has been suggested for other cancers. Basing an intervention to promote behaviour change in an explanatory framework informed by psychological theory and empirical evidence increases the likelihood that it will be effective (Bish et al, 2005; Serlachius and Sutton, 2009).

To impact on presentation with cancer symptoms, we need a greater understanding of the psychological and sociological factors influencing patients’ help-seeking behaviour. In addition, we need to devise culturally sensitive strategies, not only to improve awareness of cancer, but also to aid interpretation of symptom seriousness by patients. Ideally, an intervention to reduce delayed presentation of cancer would promote early help-seeking behaviour by people with a high cancer risk, but would not promote anxiety among those at low risk. It is important that patients are neither made unnecessarily anxious, nor should general practitioners be overburdened by consultations with ‘the worried well’. The demographic risk factors for patient delay and low cancer awareness suggest the population groups that we should target with interventions to promote early presentation. The evidence suggests a general focus on populations that are socially deprived and less educated, as well as those from black and minority ethnic groups. Interventions related to breast cancer should be targeted at older women. However, further research is needed to clarify relevant risk factors for delayed time to presentation for some particularly under-researched cancers, such as lung and prostate.

When people do present to their doctor with potential cancer symptoms, timely investigation and onwards referral are clearly important. The reviews reported here suggest that patients with particular demographic profiles are likely to experience practitioner delay, including, for some cancer sites, women and those with lower educational attainment. There is also evidence that vague and atypical symptoms are not only associated with patient delays, but also with practitioner delays. The challenge is that among the hundreds of patients with potential cancer symptoms that every general practitioner sees each year, only eight of these will actually have the disease. This review also suggests the need for effective use of referral guidelines for general practitioners, as well as better use of, and access to, diagnostic services.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.