Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Variability in patient preferences for participating in medical decision making: implication for the use of decision support tools
Free
  1. A Robinson, MRC training fellow,
  2. R Thomson, professor of epidemiology and public health
  1. Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, School of Health Sciences, Medical School , Newcastle upon Tyne NE2 4HH, UK
  1. Dr A Robinson angela.robinson{at}newcastle.ac.uk

Abstract

While there is an increasing emphasis on patient empowerment and shared decision making, evidence suggests that many patients do not wish to be involved in decisions about their own care. Previous research has found patient preferences for involvement in decision making to vary with age, socioeconomic status, illness experience, and the gravity of the decision. Furthermore, there is evidence that certain patients may experience disutility from being involved in decision making about the treatment of their health problems. We discuss the implications of these findings for the use of decision support tools and the difficulties of targeting their use towards those patients most likely to benefit. We argue that patients may be ill informed about what participation in decision making actually entails and unaware of the benefits they stand to gain by articulating their preferences to their clinician. Furthermore, clinicians are not good at accurately assessing patients' preferences, while patients may have unrealistic expectations about their clinician's ability to “know what is best” for them. Further research is required to understand variations in patients' preferences for information and involvement in decision making, and the factors that influence them.

  • patient preference
  • informed choice
  • decision making
  • patient-caregiver communication

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Key messages

  • There is increasing emphasis on patient autonomy and active involvement in decision making.

  • Evidence suggests that patients' preferences for receiving information (information evaluation) on treatments and for taking responsibility for treatment decisions varies.

  • Factors associated with these preferences include age, sex, and type of clinical problem.

  • Clinicians are poor at assessing their patients' preferences for involvement in decision making.

  • Equally, patients may not appreciate the implications of involvement in decision making and their preferences may reflect this lack of understanding or reflect past experience of health care.

  • These features have potentially profound implications for which patients should be involved in decision making, at what level, and how.

  • Further research is required to understand better patients' preferences for involvement in decision making and the factors that may influence them.

There has recently been an increased desire to involve patients more fully in decisions about their own care, a trend best illustrated by the rapidly growing literature on informed1 and shared2 decision making. Furthermore, international health policy, including that in the UK, is increasingly emphasising the policy goal of better engaging patients in health care.3 While studies have shown that better informed patients are more likely to comply with treatment,4 to be more satisfied and less anxious,56 and to have improved outcomes,710 Coulter notes that evidence for the benefits of patient participation in treatment decision making is “sparse”.11

Deber discusses the apparent discrepancy between the strong belief in the importance of patient autonomy and the existing empirical literature that appears to suggest that many patients may not wish to be active participants in making treatment decisions.1213 The work of Deber and others highlights the need to better understand patient preferences for participating in clinical consultations. In particular, a number of empirical studies have suggested that, even if patients wish to be informed about their condition and options for treatment, they might not wish to be actively involved in making the treatment decision.1416 This distinction between information evaluation and taking responsibility for treatment decisions may be important.

We discuss the implications of these findings for the use of decision support tools and illustrate our points with findings from our work in Newcastle where we have developed an expected utility based decision analysis tool to support shared decision making for patients in atrial fibrillation.17

The doctor-patient relationship

Before going on to examine patient preferences for information and for participation in decision making, it is necessary to consider various models of the doctor-patient interaction. One conceptual approach used in health economics to describe the doctor-patient relationship is one in which the physician acts as agent for the patient.1820 In this framework the principal (the patient) delegates authority to an agent (the doctor) to make treatment decisions on his behalf. The agency relationship arises because of the asymmetry of information between the doctor, who possesses superior medical information, and the patient, who possesses superior information on his preferences over treatment options. A doctor working as a perfect agent would make the same decision as the patient would were the patient to be party to the same clinical expertise as the doctor. However, such perfect agency relationships rarely exist in practice due to the principal's uncertainty as to the agent's preferences and a lack of incentives to uncover these fully.

Alternatively, styles of consultation may be considered to lie on a spectrum from “paternalistic”, which assumes primacy for the doctor's clinical knowledge and makes no (or little) concessions to patients' preferences, through to “informed consent” which (in its purest form) assumes the patient alone will make the treatment decision once he has been given all necessary clinical information. Somewhere between these two extremes lies the notion of shared decision making where the intention is that both the process and outcome of the treatment decision will be shared. Unlike the paternalistic model, both the informed and shared systems of decision making retain the objective of arriving at a treatment decision based on the doctor's knowledge and the patient's preferences. Suggested definitions of each are given in box 1.

Box 1. Definitions of informed and shared decisions.

“An informed decision is one where a reasonable choice is made by a reasonable individual using relevant information about the advantages and disadvantages of all the possible courses of action, in accord with the individual's beliefs.”1

“Shared decision making occurs when the doctor and patient share all stages of the decision making process simultaneously. In the purest form both doctor and patient reveal treatment preferences and both agree on the decision to implement.”21

The empirical evidence

This is not intended to be a comprehensive account of the evidence (Coulter affords a fuller account of this literature11), but draws on a range of studies that previously set out to examine patient preferences for participation in decision making using a number of different methodological approaches.1216192229 A further study by Kennelly and Bowling in this supplement adds to this literature.30

For example, Vick and Scott15 set out to re-examine the nature of the agency relationship in health care using the technique of conjoint analysis in which patients were asked to make discrete choices between pairwise comparisons of hypothetical “scenarios” which differed with respect to various attributes of the consultation. The questionnaire was distributed to 160 consecutive attendees in general practice and the results were based on 101 returned completed. The results showed that “being able to talk” was the most important attribute and that being involved in decision making was the least important, suggesting a distinction between the process of information evaluation and the responsibility for treatment decisions. Although patients preferred more information to less, only women and highly qualified respondents preferred to choose the treatment themselves. Older respondents and those with fewer qualifications were more likely to want the doctor to make the decision.

In a study carried out in the US, Strull et al14 administered questionnaires about three aspects of decision making to 210 hypertensive outpatients and to their 50 clinicians. They found that nearly half (47%) of patients preferred the clinician to make the therapeutic decisions “using all that is known about the medicines” but without the patient's participation. One third of the patients preferred the clinician to make the decision “but strongly to consider the patient's opinion”. Only 19% of the patients stated that they wished to share equally with the clinician in making the treatment decision, and 3% wanted to make the decision themselves. More highly educated patients and patients with more severe hypertension were more likely to prefer greater discussion. Clinicians were found to underestimate patients' desire for information and discussion but to overestimate patients' desire to make decisions.

The results of such studies highlight the potential importance of a number of demographic variables. Several studies have found that younger patients and those with higher education are more likely to want to be involved in decisions.14162326 Current health status and the severity of the health problem are other factors which have been found to influence attitudes towards involvement in decision making.151623 For example, Degner and Sloan reported that patients close to a life threatening event were more passive with respect to treatment decision preference than a comparison group of healthy individuals.23 In another study which made use of vignettes describing health states, preference for handing over control to the physician was found to be significantly greater for the vignette involving potential mortality (chest pain) than for the vignettes involving mainly morbidity (urinary problems) or quality of life (fertility).31 Of course, it is plausible that such findings may be confounding the age effects reported above as experience of illness and current health status are undoubtedly age dependent.

It has been suggested that the organisational aspects of the healthcare system may also have some influence on the desire for involvement in decisions. Thus, it has been argued that the desire for involvement in treatment decisions may be on the increase in all social groups alongside a decreased willingness to submit to the authority of clinicians.11

Discussion

The evidence suggests that there are a multitude of factors determining patients' desire for involvement in decision making and that there may be differences between the desire for receiving information (the process of information evaluation) and the desire to take responsibility for the treatment decision itself. Within a framework which sets out to increase patient autonomy and to involve patients more fully in their own care, it naturally follows that patient preferences over their involvement in decision making must be considered alongside their preferences over their illness or treatments for their medical condition. This is particularly important in light of suggestions that consumers of healthcare services may experience disutility from being involved in decision making about the treatment of their health problems.19 Furthermore, patient preferences on their information needs can influence their response to its provision, such that those given information who prefer not to have it may suffer greater anxiety in decision making than in the absence of such information.25

The question then becomes: how do we assess patients' preferences for participation in decision making in order to predict who will benefit from the use of decision support tools and who will not? One attempt to assess predisposition to involvement in treatment decisions is the Degner scale which was derived to characterise decision making preferences on the spectrum between paternalistic and being informed (see box 2).23 However, the Degner scale may be criticised for concentrating predominantly on the desire for responsibility for taking treatment decisions and not for information exchange. In theory, tools such as these may be used to assess preferences for participation and the amount of information given, and the structure of the consultation may be tailored to the individual patient's needs. Dowie32 uses the term “meta-preference” to describe the patient's preferences over both the nature of the doctor-patient relationship and the decisions made within that relationship. He argues that research on patient involvement in decision making should be focused on “better” decisions with respect to these meta-preferences and this will not necessarily be compatible with fulfilment of the patient's health state preferences.

Box 2. The Degner scale.

  • I prefer to make the final selection about which treatment I receive.

  • I prefer to make the final selection of my treatment after seriously considering my doctor's opinion.

  • I prefer that my doctor and I share responsibility for deciding which treatment is best for me.

  • I prefer that my doctor makes the final decision about which treatment will be used, but seriously considers my opinion.

  • I prefer to leave all decisions regarding my treatment to my doctor.

One potential problem with mechanisms such as these is that the patient is ill informed about what participation in decision making actually entails. This is overcome to some extent by the approach used by Dowie and colleagues on their current research on oophorectomy, whereby patients are asked directly whether they wish to receive information and answer preference questions (such as time trade off questions). While there is appeal in the directness of the Dowie approach, there remains the problem that patients may be ill informed about the advantages of participating in the treatment decision. In our focus group work with patients in atrial fibrillation we found an overriding belief that their GP knows enough about their preferences to make the decision that is best for them. In terms of agency theory, the patients appear to believe that the GP is acting as a perfect agent for them, even without eliciting their preferences explicitly.

This may fit with the traditional concept of a family GP who knows a patient for many years, but fits less well in the context of modern primary care and patient mobility. This perception also appears to be at odds with the evidence that doctors consistently underestimate patients' desire for information and that they are not good at eliciting patients' preferences.11 Furthermore, doctors' and patients' preferences for treatment options may vary markedly.33 For example, in a study of treatments for menorrhagia, Coulter et al34 found that, after the consultation, GPs remained unaware of the patients' treatment preference in 45% of cases. Even women expressing (in the questionnaire) a very strong preference for a treatment option appeared to have difficulty communicating this to their GP. Of course, in believing (perhaps mistakenly) that the doctor is already acting as their perfect agent, there would be little to be gained by their input into the decision. Thus, in order to make an informed decision about the extent to which they wish to participate, they must be given some indication of what the benefits of them doing so are likely to be.

Another potential problem with eliciting ex ante preferences for involvement is that many patients may have no former experience of participation in treatment decisions and, hence, their views may simply be reflecting a desire to maintain the status quo. Salkeld et al35 use the term “veil of experience” to describe this effect. This seems particularly relevant when considering the perspectives of older patients such as those in our study. Our focus group work, and subsequent piloting of a computerised decision aid, also suggest that patients may be more receptive to, and value, risk information once they have experienced its use. This also raises an associated issue of how immutable patient preferences for involvement are. While it would be surprising if people cannot change their preferences in this case, as in most others, with changing experience, there is a need to better understand this.

There is also the need to explore the reason why preferences for involvement in decision making vary between individuals or across socioeconomic groups. For example, Beisecker and colleagues offer two possible explanations of why older patients may want less input into medical decision making.27 The first is “role theory”—older patients came of age during times when the doctor was a traditional power figure, someone to be revered and obeyed. Hence, this may be an age cohort rather than an age dependent effect. The second explanation is more “developmental”—as people age they want less responsibility for treatment decisions and tend to rely more on the expertise or responsibility of others. The two explanations would appear to have quite different implications for the increased use of decision support tools in an ageing population.

Conclusion

We have provided a brief discussion of some of the issues surrounding patients' desire for involvement in decision making and illustrated these with reference to our work in Newcastle. The increased emphasis on involving patients more fully in their own care and the development of decision support tools for use in a wide range of clinical areas makes this an important area for further research. There appears to be a discrepancy between the strong belief in patient autonomy and the evidence that suggests that many patients do not wish to be active participants in making treatment decisions, even if they may be receptive to greater information exchange. It also appears that the apparent desire for a more paternalistic approach to decision making may be stronger in older and less well educated patients.

One obvious response to such findings may simply be to screen out such patients and to administer decision support tools only to those who express a desire to participate. Our own work, and that of others, suggests that this approach may be flawed as patients are often ill informed about the extent to which doctors understand their preferences and, hence, the benefits of participating more fully in the consultation. Furthermore, the preferences of patients with no prior experience of involvement in the decision may simply be reflecting the status quo. There are obvious equity issues here as better educated patients may already be more able to articulate their preferences to their clinician. The selective use of decision support tools would only appear to perpetuate any existing inequities.

It is also worth noting that decision support tools vary widely, from those that largely support better information exchange to those that seek to actively engage patients in making treatment choices. If preferences for information and for treatment responsibility differ, there may be a need to match better the use of decision aids to individual patient preferences for involvement.

We believe a more useful approach to the problem would be to understand better the motivation behind patients' views regarding their role in decision making. It seems there would be scope for multidisciplinary research in this area that made use of both quantitative techniques, such as conjoint analysis (see paper by Ryan et al36 in this supplement) and qualitative data methods (see papers by Frewer et al37 and Kennelly and Bowling30 in this supplement). Such methods may be used to explore whether apparent age related differences are truly age dependent or reflect an age cohort effect, as well as exploring other factors influencing preferences for participation in decision making. There would also appear to be a role for experimental work—for example, to test to what extent the desire for risk information or involvement in treatment decisions changes with experience of its use. Further work might be needed to clarify differences between preferences for information and for responsibility for treatment decisions, and how any such differences may affect models of the doctor-patient interaction or development and evaluation of decision support tools.

While further discussion of these issues is beyond the scope of this paper, the increasing development and use of decision support tools and the shift towards patient empowerment raise a multitude of questions worthy of detailed research in an area lacking in robust studies to date.

Acknowledgments

The work upon which this paper draws (the DARTs project) was funded by a grant from the West Midlands Regional Office of the NHS Executive and Eli Lilley. We are grateful to the Commissioning Group and, in particular, to Professor Richard Lilford for their support and guidance. We would like to thank members of the DARTS team (Jane Greenaway, Philip Lowe, David Parkin, Martin Eccles, Karen Jones, Rosie Stacy, Helen Park, Ian Purves, Bob Sugden, and Emma Hutchinson) and other colleagues, including those in the MRC HSRC workshop, who helped in formulating our thoughts.

References