Skip to main content
Log in

Feasibility of self-reflection as a tool to balance clinical reasoning strategies

  • Published:
Advances in Health Sciences Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Clinicians are believed to use two predominant reasoning strategies: system 1 based pattern recognition, and system 2 based analytical reasoning. Balancing these cognitive reasoning strategies is widely believed to reduce diagnostic error. However, clinicians approach different problems with different reasoning strategies. This study explores whether clinicians have insight into their problem specific reasoning strategy, and whether this insight can be used to balance their reasoning and reduce diagnostic error. In Experiment 1, six medical residents interpreted eight ECGs and self-reported their predominant reasoning strategy using a four point scale (4S). Self-assessed reasoning strategy correlated with objective assessment by two clinical experts using a post hoc talk-aloud protocol (ρ = 0.69, p < 0.0001). Reporting an analytic strategy was also associated with 40% longer interpretation times (p = 0.01). In Experiment 2, twenty-four residents were asked to reinterpret eight ECGs with instructions customized to their 4S. Half of the ECGs were reinterpreted with instructions to use the opposite reasoning strategy to that reported, and half with instructions to use the same reasoning strategy. ECG reinterpretation scores did not differ with potentiating compared to balancing reasoning instructions (F1,188 = 0.22, p = 0.64). However, analytic instructions were associated with improved scores (F1,188 = 15, p < 0.0001). These data suggest that clinicians are able to recognize their reasoning strategies. However, attempting to balance reasoning strategies through customizable instructions did not result in a reduction in diagnostic errors. This suggests important limitations to the widespread belief in balancing reasoning strategies to reduce diagnostic error.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ark, T., Brooks, L., & Eva, K. (2006). Giving learners the best of both worlds: Do clinical teachers need to guard against teaching pattern recognition to novices? Academic Medicine, 81, 405–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ark, T., Brooks, L., & Eva, K. (2007). The benefits of flexibility: The pedagogical value of instructions to adopt multifaceted diagnostic reasoning strategies. Medical Education, 41(3), 281–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berner, E., & Graber, M. (2008). Overconfidence as a cause of diagnostic error in medicine. American Journal of Medicine, 121, S2–S23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boltri, J., Hash, R., & Vogel, R. (2003). Are family practice residents able to interpret electrocardiograms? Advances in Health Sciences Education, 8(2), 149–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croskerry, P. (2005). The theory and practice of clinical decision making. Canadian Journal of Anesthesia, 52, R1–R8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croskerry, P. (2009). A universal model of diagnostic reasoning. Academic Medicine, 84, 1022–1028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, D., Mazmanian, P., Fordis, M., Van Harrison, R., Thorpe, K., & Perrier, L. (2006). Accuracy of physician self-assessment compared with observed measures of competence: A systematic review. Journal of American Medical Association, 296(9), 1094–1102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dory, V., Gagnon, R., & Charlin, B. (2010). Is case-specificity content-specificity? An analysis of data from extended-matching questions. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 15, 55–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dubin, D. (1967). Rapid interpretation of EKG’s. Tampa: COVER Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eslava, D., Dhillon, S., Berger, J., Homel, P., & Bergmann, S. (2009). Interpretation of electrocardiograms by first-year residents: The need for change. Journal of Electrocardiology, 42(6), 693–697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eva, K., & Cunnington, J. (2006). The difficulty with experience: does practice increase susceptibility to premature closure. Journal of Continuing Educational of Health Professionals, 26, 192–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eva, K., Hatala, R., LeBlanc, V., & Brooks, L. (2007). Teaching from the clinical reasoning literature: Combined reasoning strategies help novice diagnosticians overcome misleading information. Medical Education, 41, 1152–1158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. (2008). Dual processing accounts of cognitive reasoning, judgment and social cognition. Annals Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Todd, P. (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graber, M. (2009). Educational strategies to reduce diagnostic error: Can you teach this stuff? Advances in Health Science Education, 14, 63–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graber, M., Franklin, N., & Gordon, R. (2005). Diagnostic error in internal medicine. Archives of Internal Medicine, 165, 1493–1499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatala, R., Norman, G. R., & Brooks, L. R. (1999). Impact of a clinical scenario on accuracy of electrocardiogram interpretation. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 14(2), 126–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoyle, R., Walker, K., Thomson, G., & Bailey, M. (2007). Accuracy of electrocardiogram interpretation improves with emergency medicine training. Emergency Medicine Australasia, 19(2), 143–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kulatunga-Moruzi, C., Brooks, L., & Norman, G. (2001). Coordination of analytic and similarity-based processing strategies and expertise in dermatological diagnosis. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 13(2), 110–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeBlanc, V., Dore, K., Norman, G., & Brooks, L. (2004). Limiting the playing field: does restricting the number of possible diagnoses reduce errors due to diagnosis-specific feature identification? Medical Education, 38(1), 17–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lever, N., Larsen, P., Dawes, M., Wong, A., & Harding, S. (2009). Are our medical graduates in New Zealand safe and accurate in ECG interpretation? New Zealand Medical Journal, 122(1292), 9–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, K., Coderre, S., Mortis, G., & Mandin, H. (2007). Can concept sorting provide a reliable, valid and sensitive measure of medical knowledge structure? Advances in Health Science Education, 12, 265–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, K., Rikers, R., & Schmidt, H. (2008). Is analytic information processing a feature of expertise in medicine? Advances in Health Sciences Education, 13, 123–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, G. (2009). Dual processing and diagnostic errors. Advances in Health Science Education, 14, 37–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, G., Bordage, G., Page, G., & Keane, D. (2006). How specific is case specificity? Medical Education, 40(7), 618–623.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, G., & Eva, K. (2010). Diagnostic error and clinical reasoning. Medical Education, 44(1), 94–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pretz, J. (2008). Intuition versus analysis: Strategy and experience in complex everyday problem solving. Memory and Cognition, 36(3), 554–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redelmeler, D. (2005). The cognitive psychology of missed diagnoses. Annals of Internal Medicine, 142, 115–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Regehr, G., Cline, J., Norman, G., & Brooks, L. (1994). Effect of processing strategy on diagnostic skill in dermatology. Academic Medicine, 69(10 Suppl), S34–S36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salerno, S., Alguire, P., & Waxman, H. (2003a). Competency in interpretation of 12-lead electrocardiograms: A summary and appraisal of published evidence. Annals of Internal Medicine, 138(9), 751–760.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salerno, S., Alguire, P., & Waxman, H. (2003b). Training and competency evaluation for interpretation of 12-lead electrocardiograms: Recommendations from the American College of Physicians. Annals of Internal Medicine, 138(9), 747–750.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiff, G., Hasan, O., Abrams, R., Cosby, K., Lambert, B., Elstein, A., et al. (2009). Diagnostic error in medicine: Analysis of 583 physician-reported errors. Archives of Internal Medicine, 169(20), 1881–1887.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shojania, K. G., Burton, E. C., McDonald, K. M., & Goldman, L. (2003). Changes in rates of autopsy-detected diagnostic errors over time: A systematic review. Journal of American Medical Association, 289(21), 2849–2856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sim, J., & Wright, C. (2005). The kappa statistic in reliability studies: Use, interpretation and sample size requirements. Physical Therapy, 85, 257–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 211, 453–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matthew Sibbald.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sibbald, M., de Bruin, A.B.H. Feasibility of self-reflection as a tool to balance clinical reasoning strategies. Adv in Health Sci Educ 17, 419–429 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-011-9320-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-011-9320-5

Keywords

Navigation