Preparing to resume an interrupted task: effects of prospective goal encoding and retrospective rehearsal

https://doi.org/10.1016/S1071-5819(03)00023-5Get rights and content

Abstract

We examine people's strategic cognitive responses to being interrupted while performing a task. Based on memory theory, we propose that resumption of a task after interruption is facilitated by preparation during the interruption lag, or the interval between an alert to a pending interruption (e.g. the phone ringing) and the interruption proper (the ensuing conversation). To test this proposal, we conducted an experiment in which participants in a Warning condition received an 8-s interruption lag, and participants in an Immediate condition received no interruption lag. Participants in the Warning condition prepared more than participants in the Immediate condition, as measured by verbal reports, and resumed the interrupted task more quickly. However, Immediate participants resumed faster with practice, suggesting that people adapt to particularly disruptive forms of interruption. The results support our task analysis of interruption and our model of memory for goals, and suggest further means for studying operator performance in dynamic task environments.

Introduction

Consider a typical interruption. You are in conversation with a colleague when the phone rings. How do you respond? You might excuse yourself for a moment, pick up the phone and quickly reschedule the caller (“I’m in a meeting; can I call you back?”), then resume the interrupted conversation. If you were expecting a long and important call, you might instead reschedule your colleague (“Can I find you in your office in 20 min?”), and then simply take the call. In either case, you have taken explicit measures to prepare to resume a task (your conversation with your colleague) that was interrupted. This particular example, in which a ringing phone signals an upcoming interruption, represents a pattern that seems to be quite general. Even in an emergency, like an alarm that is not simply a drill or a prank, one might first hurry to save all modified files and only then evacuate the building.

In this paper, we offer a simple task analysis of interruptions, focusing on the preparatory opportunity afforded by the interruption lag, or the interval between an alert (e.g. the phone ringing) and the interruption proper (e.g. the phone call). We then offer a theoretical basis for supposing that people do in fact engage in preparatory cognitive activity during the interruption lag, activity analogous to the social negotiation with your colleague in the example above. We then present an experiment that supports the prediction and helps to show how people prepare—prospectively, by encoding specific goals to achieve at time of resumption, and retrospectively, by rehearsing state information from the point of interruption. Finally, we discuss future research and applications suggested by these findings.

Section snippets

A task analysis of interruption

Fig. 1 presents a timeline of task interruption followed by task resumption. The primary task is ongoing when an alert occurs, indicating a pending interruption by a secondary task.1 In terms of the telephone example, the primary

A theoretical framework for studying interruptions

A cognitive construct that seems to be highly relevant to how people process interruptions is what we will refer to as the goal, or an intention to perform some action in the future. For example, people seem quite capable of writing themselves “mental notes” to take up where they left off on an interrupted task, where such mental notes can be thought of as goals to resume that task at a particular point. The specific questions we address here concern the conditions necessary for being able to

Experiment

Our goal with this experiment was to manipulate the opportunity to prepare to resume after an interruption, and then to measure the effects of any differences in preparation across conditions. To measure preparation, we recorded verbal protocols, on the view that goal encoding and rehearsal are sufficiently deliberate or controlled that they rise to the level of inclusion in verbal reports (Ericsson and Simon, 1993). Participants in the Warning condition were given a visual alert followed by an

General discussion

In this paper, we have developed a simple but general task analysis of task interruption and resumption, along with several measures (resumption lag and disruption score) that characterize the disruptive effects of interruption. We also discussed a theoretical framework for analyzing memory for goals, and presented some concrete theory-based predictions. We tested one prediction in particular, namely that preparing before an interruption allows people to resume their primary task more quickly.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported in part by grant number N0001402WX20870 from the Office of Naval Research to Greg Trafton. We thank Paula D. Raymond, Sheryl Miller, Chris Monk, Bob Holt, Debbie Boehm-Davis and five anonymous reviewers for their comments on the manuscript.

References (37)

  • E.M Altmann et al.

    Memory for goalsan activation-based model

    Cognitive Science

    (2002)
  • A.L Patalano et al.

    Opportunistic planningbeing reminded of pending goals

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1997)
  • H.A Simon

    The functional equivalence of problem solving skills

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1975)
  • A Allport et al.

    Task-switching, stimulus–response binding, and negative priming

  • J.R Anderson

    The Architecture of Cognition

    (1983)
  • J.R Anderson et al.

    Tower of Hanoievidence for the cost of goal retrieval

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

    (2001)
  • J.R Anderson et al.

    Atomic Components of Thought

    (1998)
  • J.R Anderson et al.

    Human memoryan adaptive perspective

    Psychological Review

    (1989)
  • J.R Anderson et al.

    Reflections of the environment in memory

    Psychological Science

    (1991)
  • Ballas, J.A., Kieras, D.E., Meyer, D.E., Brock, D., Stroup, J., 1999. Cueing of display objects by 3-D audio to reduce...
  • Brandimonte, M., Einstein, G.O., McDaniel, M.A. (Eds.), 1996. Prospective Memory: Theory and Applications. Erlbaum,...
  • Brock, D., Ballas, J.A., Stroup, J.L., 2002a. Using an auditory display to manage attention in a dual task, multiscreen...
  • Brock, D., Stroup, J.L., Ballas, J.A., 2002b. Effects of 3D auditory display on dual task performance in a simulated...
  • D Brock et al.

    Cognitive representation of common ground in user interfaces

  • E.R.F.W Crossman

    A theory of the acquisition of speed skill

    Ergonomics

    (1959)
  • Cutrell, E., Czerwinski, M., Horvitz, E., 2001. Notification, disruption, and memory: effects of messaging...
  • M.P Czerwinski et al.

    Interruptions in multitasking situationsthe effects of similarity and warning (JSC-24757)

    (1991)
  • Czerwinski, M.P., Cutrell, E., Horvitz, E., 2000. Instant messaging and interruption: influence of task type on...
  • Cited by (353)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text